It won't be a surprise to music fans that we show a lifelong preference
for the songs of our youth. The audience for oldies shows consists
mostly of people who were in high school or college at the time the
music was originally popular.
Several
explanations have been suggested for the bump. One possibility is
that we had many vivid first-time experiences during late adolescence
and young adulthood, which were encoded in memory more strongly due
to their emotional content. The first experience of a given type may
also become a prototype, which is more easily recalled than other
members of the category. It has also been suggested that hormonal and
neurobiological changes play a role.
In a new study by Krumhansl and Zupnick, 62 Cornell University students with an average
age of 20 were played clips from the two top hits of every year from
1955 to 2009 in random order. They were asked whether they
recognized each song, whether they liked it, and to rate its quality.
They indicated whether they had personal memories of each song, and
if so when and with whom they heard it. Here are the results for
recognition, quality, liking and personal memories.
These students are
too young to test for the reminiscence bump. Their better recall for
songs released after 2000 has a trivial alternative explanation—that
more recent songs are better remembered. However, there were
“cascading reminiscence bumps”—two earlier, smaller bumps,
which are seen most clearly in the recognition and personal memories
data. One occurs at from 1980-1984, about the time their parents
were 20, and the other in the '60s, the decade when most of their
parents were born.
The
obvious explanation for the 1980-84 bump is that as children and
adolescents, we are exposed to out parents' favorite music and wind
up liking some of it. (My parents were fans of big band
music. I didn't care much for big bands when I lived at home, but
I've gradually come to like them much better—a sleeper
effect?) The authors suggest
that the '60s bump could reflect the musical taste of the students'
grandparents. They also entertain the hypothesis that '60s music is
generally better known and of higher quality than the music of other
decades—a claim I regard as suspect.
Krumhansl and Zupnick also asked their participants what genres of music they
listened to while growing up and now. Of course, their sample was
neither large nor representative of college students generally, but
here are the results.
Neither jazz nor
blues did well—a problem for the future of both genres. I was
prepared to see them overshadowed by pop, rock and hip-hop, but they also did
worse than classical, country and soundtracks! (Of course, in recent years, the
more popular soundtracks have been collections of recent hits, rather
than original music composed for the film.)
You may also be interested in:
The Sight of Music
You may also be interested in:
The Sight of Music
No comments:
Post a Comment